The following provides a snapshot of the results from the second wave of data collected in Spain for the project ‘establishing the social licence for tracking technologies’. Data was collected from August 8th - August 12th 2020. Click here to get the code and functions for this analysis, or click to see the international results.
The COVID-19 pandemic caused by the severe acute respiratory coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) disease has changed how people live, work and socialise. In the absence of a vaccine or treatment, behavioral measures such as restricting public gatherings and physical distancing, masking wearing, lockdown policies and hand-washing have been employed to arrest the spread of the virus. Spain has been severely affected by COVID-19; the virus spread rapidly and claimed many lives early in the pandemic, and now as restrictions have eased, Spain is experiencing a second-wave of infections (see Figure 1). The highly transmittable and often asymptomatic nature of this virus (appx 15% of all cases) has required new technological solutions to curve its spread. Smartphone tracking technologies and immunity passports offer two such solutions.
In this study, we asked a sample of the Spanish public about their perceptions of the COVID-19 pandemic, their perceptions about their Government (and other Governments) responses to the pandemic, their attitudes towards and uptake of the newly released ‘COVID Radar’ app, and their attitudes towards the introduction of an immunity passport. The questions and results are as follows.
Bayesian ordinal probit regressions were used to compare Likert-style responses using the MCMCoprobit and HPDinterval functions in R, taken from the MCMCpack (Martin, Quinn and Park, 2011) and Coda (Plummer, Best, Cowles and Vines, 2006) packages, respectively. Bayesian credible intervals were calculated for binomial distributions (e.g., yes or no responses), using the bayes.prop.test function from the BayesianFirstAid package (Bååth, 2014).
These Bayesian methods sample a posterior distribution of plausible means (the probability that, given our data, the true population mean is x), by weighing the likelihood of a given observation against its prior probability of occurring in the sample. Under simple parametric assumptions about the posterior distribution, these posterior distributions act to constrain the effect of outliers in the tails of the sampled data, and allow the highest region of data density — credible regions of the data distribution — to inform policy decisions. Practically, this means instead of testing a threshold of significance (like p-values), we may instead directly compare the 95% credible regions of the data distributions to determine if they do not overlap, and therefore, differ in a significantly meaningful fashion.
The MCMCoprobit function was run with 20,000 Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) iterations (including 1000 burn-ins) and a tuning parameter of 0.3 (corresponding to the size of the Metropolis-Hastings step). Default priors were used for all parameters (i.e., the distributional mean and the cutpoints), corresponding to a uniform improper prior for both the mean and the cutpoints. The bayes.prop.test function was run with 20,000 MCMC iterations (including 1000 burn-ins). Default priors were again used: a beta distribution with parameters of α = 1 and β = 1, corresponding to a uniform prior over the range [0, 1]. Ninety-five percent highest posterior density intervals (HPDIs) were estimated on the resulting posterior samples, and significant differences between items were decided where the HPDIs did not overlap.
We sampled 1086 Spanish residents who were screened for being aged 18 or older, for passing a scenario comprehension ‘attention check’, and for completing the survey (see Table 1). The final participant sample was 618 (NaN% female). Participants most frequently reported having a university education (45%) and ages ranged from 18 years to 100 years (M = 46.8349515, SD = 15.6327983 years.) Ages were roughly uniformly distributed between 20 - 70 years, and under represented for years 70+ (see Figure 2), and a majority of the sample (75%) reported an income less-than 25,000 €.
Of 618 participants, 18 (3%) reported that they had tested positive with COVID-19, and 285 (46%) indicated that they knew someone who had tested positive with COVID-19. 23% of participants had lost their job due to COVID-19.
Initial Sample | Under 18 | Failed Attention Check | Incomplete | Final Sample |
---|---|---|---|---|
1086 | 53 | 415 | 0 | 618 |
Gender identification: Percentages | |
Percent | |
---|---|
Gender | |
Men | 48.9 |
Women | 51.1 |
Other | |
Prefer not to say | |
#Total cases | 618 |
Level of education: Percentages | |
Percent | |
---|---|
Education | |
Basic studies | 11.2 |
Bachelor/COU | 21.2 |
FP | 23.0 |
University degree | 44.7 |
#Total cases | 618 |
Income: Percentages | |
Percent | |
---|---|
Data$Wealth | |
Prefer not to answer | 1.3 |
Less than 5,000 € | 31.6 |
Between 5,000 and 24,999 € | 44.8 |
Between 25,000 and 49,999 € | 7.3 |
Between 50,000 and 74,999 € | 2 |
Between 75,000 € and 99,999 € | 5.7 |
100,000 € or more | 7.3 |
#Total cases | 614 |
Participants were asked from what sources they receive their information about COVID-19, and how much they trust these sources. Responses were made on a 5 point likert scale. The questions read:
Figure 3.a displays the likert-style response distributions classifying which informaton source participants received COVID-19 information from (light blue), and the level of trust they have in these sources (dark blue). Figure 3.b displays the mean posterior distribution from a bayesian ordinal regression conducted separately for each item. Error bars are 95% highest density intervals and significant differences within the measures of information source or level of trust can be determed by where the error bars do not overlap.
Results show that participants recieved most of their COVID-19 information from TV and frends/family, then news media and government announcements, followed by social networks, and finally radio and other sources.
Partcipants trusted Friends and Family and News media the most, followed by TV and radio, and finally Goverment announcements and social networks.
Particiants were then asked:
Participants viewed COVID-19 as ‘very’ severe for the general population, and as ‘somewhat’ to ‘very’ severed for themselves (Figure 4). They reported being ‘somewhat’ to ‘very’ concerned at becoming infected with COVID-19, or others becoming infected with COVID-19.
We asked participants to report their estimates on the number of fatalities across a range of countries with moderate-to-high media coverage in Spain. Responses were made on a sliding scale ranging from 0 - 200,000; results are reported in estimated deaths per 1000 (Figure 5).
Participants were then asked to determine how helpful Government guidelines have been during the COVID-19 pandemic. The questions were:
Results are shown in Figure 6.
Participants were asked “Overall, how well do you think the governments of the following countries have managed the COVID-19 pandemic so far?” Responses were made on 5 point likert-scale ranging from ‘not good’ to ‘extremely good’.
Participants were presented with a scenario description of the newly released ‘COVID Radar’ app for tracking the spread of COVID-19 in Spain. The scenario read as:
“The COVID-19 pandemic has quickly become a global threat. Containing the spread of the virus is essential to minimize the impact on the health system and the economy, and to save many lives. The Spanish government has proposed the use of a contact tracing application for current smartphones, to help inform people if they have been exposed to others with COVID-19. The app uses technology from Apple and Google, and could help reduce the spread of COVID-19 in the community by allowing people to voluntarily isolate themselves. When two people are close to each other, their phones would connect via Bluetooth and exchange randomly generated codes. If a person is later known to be infected, people who have been close to them then receive a notification, without the government knowing who they are. The random codes would be stored on the users’ phone and would be removed after 14 days. Also, the random identifiers that have been sent to the application server by users diagnosed with COVID-19 would be removed after 14 days. Both the use of the application and the communication of a possible contagion would be completely voluntary. The people who receive the notification would not be informed who has tested positive. No user can be identified or located, because the application does not request, use or store personal data. In no case are the movements of the users tracked, and therefore geolocation would not be possible. The application is currently available to the autonomous communities that wish to request it.”
Immediately following this scearnio, participants were asked a scenario comprehension (attention) check, followed by items probing their attitudes towards the risks and benefits of using of the proposed app. Finally, participants were asked if they were aware of and were currently using the Radar COVID app. For plotting purposes, the benefits and risks of the proposed app will be presented separately, and the first and final two questions will be presented together showing app acceptance, app awareness and app downloads.
Figure 8 displays responses to the following items assessing the beneifts of the proposed tracking technology:
Figure 9 displays responses to the following items assessing the perceived risks and harms of downloading the proposed tracking technology.
Figure 10 displays responses to the following items:
Participants were asked their views on “immunity passports” which were explained as follows:
“An ‘immunity passport’ indicates that you have had a disease and that you have antibodies to the virus that causes the disease. Having the antibodies means that you are now immune and therefore cannot spread the virus to other people. Therefore, if an antibody test indicates that you have had the disease, you could be given an ‘immunity passport’ that would later allow you to move freely. Immunity passports have been proposed as a potential step to lift movement restrictions during the COVID-19 pandemic.”
Participants then responded to the following items:
The following table summarizes results for how much people supported the introduction of Immunity Passports: 27% of participants did not support immunity passports at all, and 19% showed a lot or full support.
#Total | |
---|---|
Final support for Immunity Passports | |
Not at all | 26.8 |
Slightly | 14.8 |
A bit | 19.2 |
Moderately | 19.7 |
A lot | 11.3 |
Fully | 8.2 |
#Total cases | 609 |
Finally, participants were asked about their world views with three items assessing their libertarian attitudes. The following presents a composite measure of these world view items against a composite score of their perceived risk of COVID-19. World view items were:
##
## Pearson's product-moment correlation
##
## data: Data$WorldView and Data$COVIDrisk
## t = 0.16104, df = 607, p-value = 0.8721
## alternative hypothesis: true correlation is not equal to 0
## 95 percent confidence interval:
## -0.07295172 0.08594210
## sample estimates:
## cor
## 0.006536448